Early Subitizing Development: The Role of Visuospatial Working Memory
##plugins.themes.bootstrap3.article.main##
The present study explored enumeration processes, serial counting and subitizing, in early childhood. Subitizing is a fast and accurate assessment of small quantities. Early quantitative processing is built off of a “parallel individuation” system, a domain-general, visuospatial system that allows infants to track small sets of objects. This system creates a precise mental model of every object in a small set, similar to subitizing. Subitizing ability helps children to acquire mature symbolic representation of numeral, and deficits in enumeration processes is associate to math disabilities. However, the role of development in enumeration processes and the role of visuospatial working memory is enumeration processes is largely unknown. Hence, in the present study, we tested visuospatial working memory’s role in subitizing development in preschoolers. We tested 3 to 5-year-old’s performance in enumeration and visuospatial working memory tasks. Performance in the subitizing range (one to four items) related to individual differences in visuospatial working memory and age, while performance for greater numericities in the serial counting range did not. Interestingly, controlling for visuospatial working memory span reduced the developmental effect of subitizing, suggesting that both visuospatial working memory and age can affect individual differences in subitizing in young children, which has implications for early childhood education.
-
Akin, O., & Chase, W. (1978). Quantification of three-dimensional structures. Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance, 4(3), 397-410.
|
Google Scholar
-
Anobile, G., Cicchini, G. M., & Burr, D. C. (2016). Number As a Primary Perceptual Attribute: A Review. Perception, 45(1-2), 5-31. doi: 10.1177/0301006615602599
|
Google Scholar
-
Ashkenazi, S., Rosenberg-Lee, M., Metcalfe, A. W. S., Swigart, A. G., & Menon, V. (2013). Visuo–spatial working memory is an important source of domain-general vulnerability in the development of arithmetic cognition. Neuropsychologia, 51(11), 2305-2317. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.06.031
|
Google Scholar
-
Carey, S. (2001). Cognitive Foundations of Arithmetic: Evolution and Ontogenisis. Mind & Language, 16(1), 37-55. doi: 10.1111/1468-0017.00155
|
Google Scholar
-
Carey, S. (2004). Bootstrapping & the origin of concepts. Daedalus, 133, 59+.
|
Google Scholar
-
Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (1994). Dissociable mechanisms of subitizing and counting: neuropsychological evidence from simultanagnosic patients. Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance, 20(5), 958-975.
|
Google Scholar
-
Feigenson, L., Carey, S., & Hauser, M. (2002). The representations underlying infants' choice of more: object files versus analog magnitudes. Psychol Sci, 13(2), 150-156.
|
Google Scholar
-
Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S., & Spelke, E. . (2004). Core systems of number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(7), 307-314. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.002
|
Google Scholar
-
Formoso, J., Barreyro, J. P., Jacubovich, S., & Injoque-Ricle, I. (2017). Possible Associations between Subitizing, Estimation and Visuospatial Working Memory (VSWM) in Children. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 20. doi: 10.1017/sjp.2017.23
|
Google Scholar
-
Frye, D., Braisby, N., Lowe, J., Maroudas, C., & Nicholls, J. (1989). Young Children's Understanding of Counting and Cardinality. Child Development, 60(5), 1158-1171. doi: 10.2307/1130790
|
Google Scholar
-
Gallistel, C. R., & Gelman, R. (1992). Preverbal and verbal counting and computation. Cognition, 44(1-2), 43-74.
|
Google Scholar
-
Gallistel, C. R., & Gelman, R. . (1990). The what and how of counting. Cognition, 34(2), 197-199. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(90)90043-J
|
Google Scholar
-
Gelman, R., & Meck, E. (1983). Preschoolers' counting: principles before skill. Cognition, 13(3), 343-359.
|
Google Scholar
-
Gray, S. A., & Reeve, R. A. (2014). Preschoolers' Dot Enumeration Abilities Are Markers of Their Arithmetic Competence. PLOS ONE, 9(4), e94428. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0094428
|
Google Scholar
-
Halberda, J., & Feigenson, L. (2008). Developmental change in the acuity of the "number sense": The approximate number system in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and adults. Developmental Psychology, 44(5), 1457-1465. doi: 10.1037/a0012682
|
Google Scholar
-
Hannula-Sormunen, M. M., Lehtinen, E., & Räsänen, P. (2015). Preschool Children’s Spontaneous Focusing on Numerosity, Subitizing, and Counting Skills as Predictors of Their Mathematical Performance Seven Years Later at School. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 17(2-3), 155-177. doi: 10.1080/10986065.2015.1016814
|
Google Scholar
-
Jansen, B. R., Hofman, A. D., Straatemeier, M., van Bers, B. M., Raijmakers, M. E., & van der Maas, H. L. (2014). The role of pattern recognition in children's exact enumeration of small numbers. Br J Dev Psychol, 32(2), 178-194. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12032
|
Google Scholar
-
Landerl, K., Bevan, A., & Butterworth, B. (2004). Developmental dyscalculia and basic numerical capacities: a study of 8–9-year-old students. Cognition, 93(2), 99-125. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2003.11.004
|
Google Scholar
-
Le Corre, M., & Carey, S. (2007). One, two, three, four, nothing more: An investigation of the conceptual sources of the verbal counting principles. Cognition, 105(2), 395-438. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.10.005
|
Google Scholar
-
Le Corre, M., Van de Walle, G., Brannon, E. M., & Carey, S. (2006). Re-visiting the competence/performance debate in the acquisition of the counting principles. Cognitive Psychology, 52(2), 130-169. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2005.07.002
|
Google Scholar
-
Leibovich, T., Henik, A., & Salti, M. (2015). Numerosity processing is context driven even in the subitizing range: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 77(Supplement C), 137-147. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.08.016
|
Google Scholar
-
Leibovich-Raveh, T., Lewis, D. J., Kadhim, S. A. R., & Ansari, D. (2018). A new method for calculating individual subitizing ranges. Journal of Numerical Cognition, 4(2), 429-447..
|
Google Scholar
-
Mandler, G., & Shebo, B. J. (1982). Subitizing: an analysis of its component processes. Journal of experimental psychology. General, 111(1), 1-22.
|
Google Scholar
-
Piazza, M., Facoetti, A., Trussardi, A. N., Berteletti, I., Conte, S., Lucangeli, D., . . . Zorzi, M. (2010). Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental dyscalculia. Cognition, 116(1), 33-41. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.03.012
|
Google Scholar
-
Piazza, M., Fumarola, A., Chinello, A., & Melcher, D. (2011). Subitizing reflects visuo-spatial object individuation capacity. Cognition, 121(1), 147-153. doi: DOI 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.05.007
|
Google Scholar
-
Piazza, M., Mechelli, A., Butterworth, B., & Price, C. J. (2002). Are Subitizing and Counting Implemented as Separate or Functionally Overlapping Processes? NeuroImage, 15(2), 435-446. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0980
|
Google Scholar
-
Piper, B. J., Li, V., Eiwaz, M. A., Kobel, Y. V., Benice, T. S., Chu, A. M., . . . Mueller, S. T. (2011). Executive function on the Psychology Experiment Building Language tests. Behavior Research Methods, 44(1), 110-123. doi: 10.3758/s13428-011-0096-6
|
Google Scholar
-
Purpura, D. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (2015). Early Numeracy Assessment: The Development of the Preschool Early Numeracy Scales. Early Education and Development, 26(2), 286-313. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2015.991084
|
Google Scholar
-
Revkin, S. K., Piazza, M., Izard, V., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2008). Does subitizing reflect numerical estimation? Psychological Science, 19(6), 607-614.
|
Google Scholar
-
Sarnecka, B. W., & Gelman, S. A. (2004). Six does not just mean a lot: preschoolers see number words as specific. Cognition, 92(3), 329-352. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2003.10.001
|
Google Scholar
-
Simon, T. J., Peterson, S., Patel, G., & Sathian, K. (1998). Do the magnocellular and parvocellular visual pathways contribute differentially to subitizing and counting? Percept Psychophys, 60(3), 451-464.
|
Google Scholar
-
Slusser, E., Ditta, A., & Sarnecka, B. (2013). Connecting numbers to discrete quantification: a step in the child's construction of integer concepts. Cognition, 129(1), 31-41. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.05.011
|
Google Scholar
-
Starkey, P., & Cooper, R. (1995). The development of subitizing in young children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13(4), 399-420. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.1995.tb00688.x
|
Google Scholar
-
Szűcs, D. (2016). Chapter 11 - Subtypes and comorbidity in mathematical learning disabilities: Multidimensional study of verbal and visual memory processes is key to understanding. In C. Marinella & F. Wim (Eds.), Progress in Brain Research (Vol. Volume 227, pp. 277-304): Elsevier.
|
Google Scholar
-
Szucs, D., Devine, A., Soltesz, F., Nobes, A., & Gabriel, F. (2013). Developmental dyscalculia is related to visuo-spatial memory and inhibition impairment. Cortex, 49(10), 12. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.06.007
|
Google Scholar
-
Trick, L. M., & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1993). What enumeration studies can show us about spatial attention: evidence for limited capacity preattentive processing. Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance, 19(2), 331-351.
|
Google Scholar
-
Wynn, K. (1990). Children's understanding of counting. Cognition, 36(2), 155-193.
|
Google Scholar
Search Panel
Most read articles by the same author(s)
-
Sarit Ashkenazi,
Hagar Eisner,
Problem-Solving among Math Anxious Individuals: The Role of Advanced Strategy and Testing of Online Anxiety , European Journal of Education and Pedagogy: Vol. 3 No. 4 (2022)